Class 14: Studying the far right

Methods, ethics, and safety

Opening notes

Presentation groups

Date Presenters Method
3 July: Alexander V., Luis G., Oscar O., Mia C. descriptive inference
10 July: Lina S., Stephen W., Philomena B., Aarón Z. ethnography
17 July: Corinna Z., Eva M., and Rostislav N. process tracing
24 July: Sebastian K., Thomas R., Emilia Z., Florian P. quant. text analysis
24 July: Lorenz F., Daniel B., Medina H. quant. text analysis
Presentations line-up
Date Presenters Method
15 May: Idil M., Zeynep P., Liesl W., Selin K., Chiara W. logistic regression
22 May: Gabriel W., Lina M., Florian S., Julian B. discourse analysis
29 May: NO CLASS MEETING
Date Presenters Method
5 June: Rasmus B., Andre D., Josefine E., Ioanna L., Santiago C. surveys
12 June: Omar B., Lela E., Niclas W. network analysis
19 June: NO CLASS MEETING
26 June: Colombe I., Konstantin S., Jakob W., Veronika L. ethnography
26 June: Maksim K., Felix S., Jon L.D., Damir S., Korbinian M. case study

Approach and Methodology points to consider

  • fundamental research approach points
  • methods of studying the far right encountered in this course
  • conceptualisation example from Meijers and Zaslove (2021)
  • helpful texts on researching the far right

Approaches to studying the far right

  • selection of topic: theory-driven, method-driven, problem-/empirics-driven
  • purpose: describing, explaining, predicting
  • use of theory: testing (e.g., puzzles, Day and Koivu 2019) or generating
  • concepts: how much is constructed (i.e., new conceptualisation)?
  • methods:
    • ontological assumptions: probabilism, determinism, constructivism
    • methodological alignment (Hall 2003)
      • choice of cases: sampling or casing (Soss 2018)?
      • analysis: by variables (generalisation) or by cases (contextualisation)?

Methods encountered in this course and their uses

  • regression and other inferential statistics - probabilities/ correlations of large-N data; ‘reg. table is a statement of averages’
  • quantitative text analysis - topics/discourse, sentiment/emotion
  • experiments - the gold standard of causation; individual behaviour, survey experiments, and ‘natural experiments’
  • social network analysis (SNA) - relational aspects, shape and density and even nature of connections and flows between them
  • qualitative comparative analysis (QCA) - complex causation, conditions in necessary and/or sufficient combinations
  • case studies - diverse uses: theory-building, theory-testing, exploratory and descriptive inference, process tracing
  • discourse analysis - narratives, imagery, communication strategy
  • ethnography - many uses, which we’ll discuss in a moment

conceptualising ‘populism’ (Meijers and Zaslove 2021)

four criteria: (i) construct validity, (ii) multi-dimensionality, (iii) measurement precision and (iv) coverage

  • construct validity: capture all relevant components
    • 5 for populism: (1) sovereignty of ‘the people’, (2) ‘the people’ are homogenous, (3) ‘the people’s’ interests are united by a general will, (4) elite portrayed as corrupt, and (5) conflict of ‘the people’ and elite as a moral struggle between good and bad
  • multi-dimensionality: not only all components, measure should also capture populism’s components separately
  • measurement precision: degree differences of a concept
  • coverage: measure covers as many cases as possible

conceptualisation (Adcock and Collier 2001)

  • key tip for thesis work: be clear, systematic, and consistent on your central concepts
  • research from this course is at its best when you can easily pick out key concepts

books on researching the far right

Ashe et al. (2020) - Researching the Far Right: Theory, Method and Practice

Vaughan et al. (2024) - The Ethics of Researching the Far Right

Poll: ethical issues of studying FR

A QR code for the survey.

Take the survey at https://forms.gle/cfFFMvuHiCWJG4nF7

  • safe and reliable interview research with far-right actors is not possible
  • covert research of the far right is unethical
  • publication that encourages violent action against far-right actors
  • inform law enforcement if researcher discovers a crime
  • do not present text, images, content produced by far-right actors because it could benefit far-right actors
  • engage with policymakers whenever invited
  • govs. should minimise surveillance because the threat from state overreach, ‘authoritarian creep,’ is a more pressing concern
  • should not interview FR actors

covert research of FR is unethical

Research findings and dissemination

findings should not justify violence

researchers should report crimes

research should not publish FR content

Consequences of research

researchers should always engage with policymakers

minimise surveillance of FR, avoid ‘authoritarian creep’

Ethical research

  • ethics approval
  • problematic examples:
    • Zurich online manipulation of ChangeMyView reddit
    • Shoshan (2016) The Management of Hate

Common ethics approval points

  1. Will individuals participate in this study who belong to a vulnerable group … or who cannot give their own consent to participate (e.g., under the age of 16)?

  2. Will it be necessary that people participate in this study without having been informed about this previously or without having given their consent to participate (e.g., as in covert observation)?

  3. Will the study involve covert observation or any other method that precludes informed consent, full debriefing, or the opportunity for participants to have their data deleted?

Common ethics approval points

  1. Will the study feature questions about topics that are of an intimate nature or that participants may perceive as stigmatising (e.g., questions pertaining to illegal or deviant behaviour or to sexual preferences)?

  2. Will video or audio recordings be taken without prior consent by the participants?

  3. Which personal data, if any, will be collected? … How will the anonymisation or pseudonymisation of collected data be ensured? … How long will the data be kept and where will they be stored?

What not to do … research design

  • team at University of Zürich (UZH) used AI-generated content to ‘participate’ on ChangeMyView subreddit, testing if AI was better at changing people’s minds than humans
    • AI tailored arguments to individuals by inferring demographic features (sex, ethnicity, etc.) from prior posts
    • AI adopted false guises, e.g., male rape survivor, a trauma counselor, and a Black person who disagreed with BLM movement

What not to do … research design

  • experiment ran for four months, researchers then informed subreddit moderators, who informed users
  • ethics approval obtained, but it could not prohibit the study; ‘role was only advisory’ …
  • danger of undermining scientific credibility

What not to do … reporting

  • Shoshan (2016) provides a study of far-right youth in Köpenick district of Berlin
    • author is of Jewish ancestry, so adopting a pseudonym was necessary for research to proceed
    • author was embedded with ‘street social workers’, effectively masquerading as one of them
      • no informed consent, youth participants, covert observation, possibly illegal activity
  • There may be defensible, ethically approvable reasons for all of this—but it is not reported in the book

The wonderful work of Kathy Blee

  • ethnography of the far right
    • challenges and opportunities
  • emotions in fieldwork
  • study of (U.S.) women in far-right groups
  • transparency (e.g., Jacobs et al. 2021)

Kathy Blee’s books

1991

1998

2003

Blee (2007) on ethnography of far right

  • many studies of far right offer the view from outside: economic, social, attitudinal, or cultural environments that nurture organised racism and right-wing extremism—not internal dynamics
    • Limitations of such externalist accounts: struggles to explain movement/party emergence and participation
  • Ethnography offers solutions
    • common objects of research: political rituals, nature and intensity of FR leaders and groups, and how individual respond
    • includes techniques like life-history interviews with activists, focus groups, observation and/or participation at far-right meetings and events
    • strong in drawing attention researcher positionality problems

Blee (2007) on ethnography of FR - challenges

  • mutual mistrust:
    • researchers sometimes fail to scrutinise motives or actions by sympathetic movements/activists—the far right, conversely, is usually regarded with (excessive) scepticism
      • few researchers want to develop rapport (often integral to ethnography) with FR subjects
    • far right often regard academics as untrustworthy or hostile, as “potential agents of an enemy state (police, infiltrators, informants), spies from rival groups, or general antagonists who will expose their operations and cause harm
      • FR individuals sometimes intimidate or threaten researchers
  • This circular mutual mistrust fuels the problem of access

Blee (2007) on ethnography of FR - opportunities

  • ethnography has mostly studied progressive movements and other groups with whom researchers sympathise
  • close-up, ethnographic (and related) approaches can identify beliefs and motivations of FR activists and supporters
    • for example, emotions: ‘hostility, persecution, and anger certainly are generated and reinforced in the far right, but so are feelings of pride, amusement, and sensuality’. And there is considerable variations between movements.
  • sometimes, under certain conditions and with strict rules, researchers may covertly or discretely study FR

Blee (1998) on emotions in fieldwork with the far right

  • Fieldwork is emotional: usually both positive and negative emotions are affecting researchers
    • challenging to recognise → demanding committed and discerning introspection
    • unrecognised, can create serious problems of bias (and others) for research work
    • plus, personal consequences for researcher well-being
  • Emotions in fieldwork are negotiated and relational (not individual)

Blee (1998) on emotions in fieldwork with the far right

  • YET emotional dimensions of fieldwork can be useful:
    1. emotional dynamics between respondent and researcher can be analysed,
    2. researcher’s emotions are good data in themselves, so keep an ‘emotional log’ (what is respondent trying to evoke? sympathy, understanding, fear, aversion to transgressive/provocative statements? how is the interviewer responding?)
  • empathy, rapport, and trust with sympathetic respondents; (mutual) fear with ‘unloved groups’
    • My fear of being harmed and the respondents’ fears of being exposed…”

Background of Blee’s studies

  • research aim: the role of women within active self-defined racist and antisemitic groups operating in the U.S. in the 1994-1995.
  • data:
    • unstructured life history interviews
    • structured questionnaires
    • 34 women who were active members of a racist group
    • plus, an analysis of propaganda published by more than 100 then active racist and antisemitic groups.

Blee’s article (and other output from this project) is a masterful display of research transparencytake inspiration for your thesis!

Methods point: Blee’s justification for life histories

  • Blee (1998, 387) states three reasons
    1. avoid the tendency of racist activists to substitute organisational doctrine for personal belief in standard interview settings (as in news interviews)
      • differentiating between individuals and groups, avoiding skewed/misleading data
    2. wanted to get narratives for causation of respondents’ personal development
      • method to suit research aim
    3. life histories and narratives, rather than on the beliefs, to minimize the potential for this study to be used to broadcast racist ideologies to new audiences

Blee (1998) - finding respondents

  • locating interview respondents:
    • snowball sampling not possible: though common in studies of ‘hidden communities,’ practically, “animosity among racist groups meant that members could not be relied upon to suggest respondents from other groups” (recognition of internal dynamics of study population)

Blee (1998) - introductory interview communication

I made it clear that I did not share the racial convictions of these groups. I explicitly said that my ideological views were quite opposed to theirs, that they should not have any hope of converting me to their views, but that I would try to present an accurate depiction of women racist activists. This stance—as distant but not neutral researcher—was intended to clarify the nature of my interest in racist activists and their movement. It also positioned me as an observer who had not decided in advance to depict them as crazy or as personally pathological (a common media portrayal) and thus increase their vulnerability to incarceration by law enforcement or mental health agencies.

Blee (1998) - introductory interview communication

I made it clear that I did not share the racial convictions of these groups. I explicitly said that my ideological views were quite opposed to theirs, that they should not have any hope of converting me to their views, but that I would try to present an accurate depiction of women racist activists. This stance—as distant but not neutral researcher—was intended to clarify the nature of my interest in racist activists and their movement. It also positioned me as an observer who had not decided in advance to depict them as crazy or as personally pathological (a common media portrayal) and thus increase their vulnerability to incarceration by law enforcement or mental health agencies.

  • explains positionality, interest, and intent (no whiff of deception)
  • doing no harm
  • plus, openness during interviews to explain position further, even if respondents were uninterested (Blee 1998, 386)

Blee (1998) - arranging interview, negotiating setting

  • asked respondents: choose a comfortable place but without likelihood of interruption
    • refused on two occasions (for safety reasons): (1) get blindfolded and transported to unknown destination in back of a truck, (2) meeting in a remote racist compound, driven by a racist group member
  • interview procedures
    • reiterate positionality, interest, and intent
    • assuaging concerns: confidentiality, anonymity, and no questions about illegal activities

Blee (1998) - project duration developments

  • fear abated, numbness to extreme ideas set in
  • in one interview, ‘seductive, false rapport,’ as Blee (1998, 393) describes

Actually with Linda and [her boyfriend] there was no indication that they might try to harm me at all. In fact, quite the contrary. I actually was afraid of that before they came because they both have very violent reputations, but in person they were extremely cordial and very friendly, not trying to intimidate me in any way. Perhaps trying to cultivate me.

Blee (1998) - interview dynamics, research effects

  • respondents frequently pointed out researcher’s vulnerability (fear and intimidation)
  • acknowledge that persons from disagreeable groups can be quite agreeable respondents (emotional dissonance for researchers)
  • effects of research

To avoid giving further publicity to racist groups as well as to ensure anonymity, I use pseudonyms for both respondents’ names and the names of their groups and have changed some identifying details.

A final discussion question

Posed by a previous student of this course:

Is democracy in decline? Is the far right a symptom/cause of that? If so, then what can/should we do? Is there a possibility science can “help”?

The quotation marks around ‘help’ are artful and wonderfully wry, recalling other times when science has ‘helped’

Valedictory remarks

Thank you for your presence and participation!

Any questions, concerns, feedback for this class?

Anonymous feedback here: https://forms.gle/pisUmtmWdE13zMD58

Alternatively, send me an email: m.zeller@lmu.de

References

Adcock, Robert, and David Collier. 2001. “Measurement Validity: A Shared Standard for Qualitative and Quantitative Research.” American Political Science Review 95 (3): 529–46. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055401003100.
Ashe, Stephen D, Joel Busher, Graham Macklin, and Aaron Winter, eds. 2020. Researching the Far Right: Theory, Method and Practice. London: Routledge. https://arxiv.org/abs/1011.1669v3.
Blee, Kathleen M. 1998. “White-Knuckle Research: Emotional Dynamics in Fieldwork with Racist Activists.” Qualitative Sociology 21 (4): 381–99. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023328309725.
———. 2007. “Ethnographies of the Far Right.” Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 36 (2): 119–28. https://doi.org/10.1177/0891241606298815.
Day, Christopher, and Kendra L. Koivu. 2019. “Finding the Question: A Puzzle-Based Approach to the Logic of Discovery.” Journal of Political Science Education 15 (3): 377–86. https://doi.org/10.1080/15512169.2018.1493594.
Hall, Peter A. 2003. “Aligning Ontology and Methodology in Comparative Research.” In Comparative Historical Analysis in the Social Sciences, edited by James Mahoney and Dietrich Rueschemeyer, 373–404. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Jacobs, Alan M., Tim Büthe, Ana Arjona, Leonardo R. Arriola, Eva Bellin, Andrew Bennett, Lisa Björkman, et al. 2021. “The Qualitative Transparency Deliberations: Insights and Implications.” Perspectives on Politics, 1–38. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1537592720001164.
Meijers, Maurits J., and Andrej Zaslove. 2021. “Measuring Populism in Political Parties: Appraisal of a New Approach.” Comparative Political Studies 54 (2): 372–407. https://doi.org/10.1177/0010414020938081.
Shoshan, Nitzan. 2016. The Management of Hate: Nation, Affect, and the Governance of Right-Wing Extremism in Germany. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Soss, Joe. 2018. “On Casing a Study Versus Studying a Case.” Qualitative and Multi-Method Research 16 (1): 21–27.
Vaughan, Antonia, Meghan Tinsley, Aurelien Mondon, and Joan Braune. 2024. The Ethics of Researching the Far Right: Critical Approaches and Reflections. Manchester: Manchester University Press.